16167
Comment:
|
24996
|
Deletions are marked like this. | Additions are marked like this. |
Line 6: | Line 6: |
This page provides some general recommendations for your event-related analysis. It is not directly related with the auditory dataset, but provides guidelines you should consider for any MEG/EEG experiment. We do not provide standard analysis pipelines for resting or steady state recordings yet, but we will add a few examples soon in the section [[http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm/Tutorials#Other_analysis_scenarios|Other analysis scenarios]] of the tutorials page. | This page provides some general recommendations for your event-related analysis. It is not directly related with the auditory dataset, but provides guidelines you should consider for any MEG/EEG experiment. <<BR>>We do not provide standard analysis pipelines for resting or steady state recordings yet, but we will add a few examples soon in the section [[http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm/Tutorials#Other_analysis_scenarios|Other analysis scenarios]] of the tutorials page. |
Line 11: | Line 11: |
The most appropriate analysis pipeline for your data depends on the question you are trying to answer. What is the objective you have with your data? * Contrast two experimental conditions across trials, for '''one single subject''' (within subject) * Files A: Single trials for condition A. * Files B: Single trials for condition B. * Use independent tests, or difference of averages. * Contrast two groups of subjects for one given experimental condition (between subjects) * Files A: Averages for group of subjects #1. * Files B: Averages for group of subjects #2. * Use '''independent tests''', or difference of averages. * Contrast two experimental conditions across multiple subjects (between subjects) * Files A: All subjects, average for condition A. * Files B: All subjects, average for condition B. * Use '''paired tests''' (= dependent tests), or average of differences. What are the dimensions you want to explore? |
The most appropriate analysis pipeline for your data depends on the question you are trying to answer. Before defining what are the main steps of your analysis, you should be able to state clearly the question you want to answer with your recordings. ==== What dimension? ==== |
Line 31: | Line 15: |
* Cortical sources: | * Cortical sources |
Line 35: | Line 19: |
* Time-frequency dimensions What level of precisions you want to get? * Averages / difference of averages * Identify statistically significant differences '''[TODO: WHEN TO USE WHAT]''' |
* Frequency or time-frequency maps ==== What kind of experiment? ==== * '''Single subject''': Contrast two experimental conditions across trials, for one single subject. * Files A: Single trials for condition A. * Files B: Single trials for condition B. * '''Group analysis, within subject''': Contrast two conditions A and B measured for each subject. * Files A: Subject-level averages for condition A (all the subjects). * Files B: Subject-level averages for condition B (all the subjects). * '''Group analysis, between subjects''': Contrast two groups of subjects for one condition. * Files A: Subject-level averages for group #1 (G1). * Files B: Subject-level averages for group #2 (G2). ==== What level of precision? ==== * Difference of averages * Statistically significant differences between conditions or groups ==== What statistical test? ==== * '''A = B''' * Tests the null hypothesis H0:(A=B) against the alternative hypothesis H1:(A<<HTML(≠)>>B) * Correct detection: Identify correctly '''where and when''' the conditions are different. * Ambiguous sign: We cannot say which condition is stronger. * '''Power(A) = Power(B)''' * Tests the null hypothesis H0:(Power(A)=Power(B)) against the alternative hypothesis H1:(Power(A)<<HTML(≠)>>Power(B)) * Incorrect detection: Not sensitive to the cases where A and B have opposite signs. * Meaningful sign: We can identify correctly which condition has a '''stronger response'''. * Power(x) = |x|<<HTML(<SUP>2</SUP>)>>, where |x| represents the modulus of the values: <<BR>> - Absolute value for scalar values (recordings, constrained sources, time-frequency) <<BR>> - Norm of the three orientations for unconstrained sources. * '''Multiple comparisons''': FDR is a good choice for correcting p-values for multiple comparisons. ==== Design considerations ==== * Use within-subject designs whenever possible (i.e. collect two conditions A and B for each subject), then contrast data at the subject level before comparing data between subjects. * Such designs are not only statistically optimal, but also ameliorate the between-subject sign ambiguities as contrasts can be constructed within each subject. |
Line 45: | Line 53: |
All the event-related studies can start with the pipeline we've introduced in these beginners' tutorials. | Most event-related studies can start with the pipeline we've introduced in these tutorials. |
Line 53: | Line 61: |
* Evaluate the quality of the recordings with a power spectrum density (PSD). | * Evaluate the quality of the recordings with a power spectral density plot (PSD). |
Line 59: | Line 67: |
==== How many trials to include? ==== * '''Single subject''': Include all the good trials (unless you have a very low number of trials). <<BR>>See the [[http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm/Tutorials/Averaging#Number_of_trials|averaging tutorial]]. * '''Group analysis''': Use a similar numbers of trials for all the subjects (no need to be strictly equal), reject the subjects for which you have much less good trials. |
|
Line 61: | Line 73: |
* Average the epochs across sessions and subjects: OK. | * Average the epochs across acquisition runs: OK. * Average the epochs across subjects: OK. |
Line 63: | Line 76: |
=== Within subject statistics === * '''A vs'''''' B''': * Never use an absolute value for testing recordings. * Parametric or non-parametric tests, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Correct effect size (we identify correctly where and when the conditions are different). * Ambiguous sign (we cannot say which condition is stronger). === Between subjects statistics === * '''(A-B=0)''': Parametric or non-parametric tests, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. |
* Never use an absolute value for averaging or contrasting sensor-level data. === Statistics: Single subject === * '''A ='''''' B''': Parametric or non-parametric t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. === Statistics: Group analysis, within subject === * '''A ='''''' B''' * '''First-level statistic''': For each subject, sensor average for conditions A and B. * '''Second-level statistic''': Parametric or non-parametric t-test, '''paired''', two-tailed. === Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects === * '''A ='''''' B''' * '''First-level statistic''': For each subject, sensor average for the conditions to test. * '''Second-level statistic''': Parametric/non-parametric t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. |
Line 75: | Line 92: |
==== Average ==== * Average the epochs within each session: OK. * Averaging across sessions: Not advised because the head of the subject may move between runs. * Averaging across subjects: Strongly discouraged because the shape of the heads vary but the sensors are fixed. * Tolerance for data exploration: averaging across runs and subjects can be useful for identifying time points and sensors with interesting effects but should be avoided for formal analysis. * Note for Elekta/MaxFilter users: You can align all sessions to a reference session, this will allow direct channel comparisons within-subject. Not recommended across subjects. ==== Within subject statistics ==== * '''A = ''''''B''' * Never use an absolute value for testing recordings. * Parametric or non-parametric tests, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Correct effect size (we identify correctly where and when the conditions are different). * Ambiguous sign (we cannot say which condition is stronger). ==== Between subjects statistics ==== |
=== Average === * Average the epochs within each acquisition runs: OK. * Average across runs: Not advised because the head of the subject may move between runs. * Average across subjects: Strongly discouraged because the shape of the heads vary but the sensors are fixed. One sensor does not correspond to the same brain region for different subjects. * Tolerance for data exploration: Averaging across runs and subjects can be useful for identifying time points and sensors with interesting effects but should be avoided for formal analysis. * Note for Elekta/MaxFilter users: You can align all acquisition run to a reference run, this will allow direct channel comparisons and averaging across runs. Not recommended across subjects. * Never use an absolute value for averaging or contrasting sensor-level data. === Statistics: Single subject === * '''A ='''''' B''': Parametric or non-parametric t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. === Statistics: Group analysis === |
Line 93: | Line 107: |
=== Within-subject average === 1. '''Sensor average''': Compute one sensor-level average''' '''per acquisition session and condition (MEG/EEG). <<BR>>Use the '''same number of trials''' for all the averages. 1. '''Sources''': Estimate sources for each session average (constrained or unconstrained, no normalization). |
=== Average: Single subject === 1. '''Sensor average''': Compute one sensor-level average''' '''per acquisition run and per condition. 1. '''Sources''': Estimate sources for each average (constrained, no normalization). 1. '''Source average''': Average the source-level run averages to get one subject average.<<BR>>Compute a weighted average to balance for different numbers of trials across runs. 1. '''Low-pass filter''' your evoked responses (optional). <<BR>>If you filter the average before normalizing wrt baseline, it will lead to an underestimation of the baseline variance, and therefore to an overestimation of the Z scores computed in the next step, especially if the baseline is too short (typically less than 200 time points). The filter increases the autocorrelation of the time series, and therefore biases the estimation of the signal variance ([[https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Unbiased_estimation_of_standard_deviation#Effect_of_autocorrelation_.28serial_correlation.29|Wikipedia]]). 1. '''Normalize '''the subject min-norm averages: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value).<<BR>>Justification: The amplitude range of current densities may vary between subjects because of anatomical or experimental differences. This normalization helps bringing the different subjects to the same range of values. 1. '''Do not rectify the cortical maps''', but display them as absolute values if needed. === Average: Group analysis === 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above. 1. '''Rectify''' the cortical maps (apply an absolute value). <<BR>>Justification: Cortical maps have ambiguous signs across subjects: reconstructed sources depend heavily on the orientation of true cortical sources. Given the folding patterns of individual cortical anatomies vary considerably, cortical maps have subject-specific amplitude and sign ambiguities. This is true even if a standard anatomy is used for reconstruction. 1. '''Project '''the individual source maps on a template (only when using the individual brains). <<BR>> For more details, see tutorial: [[Tutorials/CoregisterSubjects|Group analysis: Subject coregistration]]. 1. '''Group average''': Compute grand averages of all the subjects.<<BR>>Do __not__ use a weighted average: all the subjects should have the same weight in this average. 1. '''Smooth '''spatially the source maps (optional).<<BR>>You can smooth after step #3 for computing non-parametric statistics with the subject averages. For a simple group average, it is equivalent to smooth before of after computing the average. === Difference of averages: Within subject === 1. '''Sensor average''': Compute one sensor-level average per acquisition run and condition. 1. '''Sources''': Estimate sources for each average (constrained, no normalization). |
Line 97: | Line 127: |
1. <<HTML(<FONT color="#777777">)>>'''Low-pass filter''' < 40Hz for evoked responses (optional)<<HTML(</FONT>)>> 1. '''Normalize '''the subject min-norm averages: Z-score vs. baseline (no absolute value).<<BR>>Justification: The amplitude range of current densities may vary between subjects because of anatomical or experimental differences. This normalization helps bringing the different subjects to the same range of values. 1. '''Do not rectify the cortical maps''', but display them in absolute values. === Between-subjects average === 1. '''Subject average'''s: Compute the within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described just above. 1. '''Rectify''' the cortical maps (apply an absolute value). <<BR>>Justification: Cortical maps have ambiguous signs across subjects: reconstructed sources depend heavily on the orientation of true cortical sources. Given the folding patterns of individual cortical anatomies vary considerably, cortical maps have subject-specific amplitude and sign ambiguities. This is true even if a standard anatomy is used for reconstruction. 1. '''Project '''the individual source maps on a template. 1. '''Smooth '''spatially the sources.<<BR>>Justification: The effects observed with constrained cortical maps may be artifically very focal and not overlapping very well between subjects. Smoothing the cortical maps may help the activated regions overlap between subjects. |
1. '''Subject difference''': Compute the difference between conditions for each subject #i: (A,,i,,-B,,i,,) 1. '''Low-pass filter''' the difference (optional) 1. '''Normalize '''the difference: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value): Z(A,,i,,-B,,i,,) 1. '''Rectify''' the difference (apply an absolute value): |Z(A,,i,,-B,,i,,)| 1. '''Project '''the individual difference on a template (only when using the individual brains). 1. '''Group average''': Compute grand averages of all the subjects: avg(|Z(A,,i,,-B,,i,,)|). 1. '''Smooth '''spatially the source maps (optional). === Difference of averages: Between subjects === 1. '''Grand averages''': Compute averages for groups #1 and #2 as in ''Average:Group analysis.'' 1. '''Difference''': Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(|G,,1,,|)-avg(|G,,2,,|) 1. '''Limitations''': Because we rectify the source maps before computing the difference, we lose the ability to detect the differences between equal values of opposite signs. And we cannot keep the sign because we are averaging across subjects. Therefore, many effects are not detected correctly. === Statistics: Single subject === * '''A = B''': Parametric or non-parametric * Compute source maps for each trial (constrained, no normalization). * Parametric or non-parametric two-sample t-test, two-tailed.<<BR>>Identifies correctly '''where and when''' the conditions are different (sign not meaningful). * '''Directionality''': Additional step to know which condition has higher values.<<BR>>Compute the difference of rectified averages: |avg(A,,i,,)|-|avg(B,,i,,)|<<BR>>Combine the significance level (t-test) with the direction (difference): [[http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm/Tutorials/Statistics#Directionality:_Difference_of_absolute_values|See details]]. * '''Power(A) = Power(B)''': Parametric '''[???]''' * Compute source maps for each trial (constrained, '''dSPM'''). * Parametric F-test: Trials A,,i,, and B,,i,, must be normalized (zero mean, unit variance)<<BR>>PowerA = sum(A,,i,,^2^)/N,,A,, ~ Chi2(N,,A,,), PowerB = sum(B,,j,,^2^)/N,,B,, ~ Chi2(N,,B,,)<<BR>>F = PowerA / Power ~ F(N,,A,,,N,,B,,), can be tested with the F-distribution. * '''Power(A) = 0''': Parametric '''[???]''' * Power(s(t)) = sum_trials (s(t)^2); * Power_baseline = mean_{t<0} (sum_trials(s_base(t)^2)); * F(large,large) = Power(s(t)) / Power_baseline; * But more popular statistic: Power(s(t) – power(baseline) / power(baseline)<<BR>>Do not have parametric statistic === Statistics: Group analysis, within subject === * '''Power(A-B) = 0''': Parametric * '''First-level statistic''': Rectified difference of normalized averages. <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Difference of averages: Within subjects'', but stop before the group average (after step #8). You obtain one measure '''|A,,i,,-B,,i,,|''' per subject, test these values against zero. * '''Second-level statistic''': Parametric Chi^2^-test. <<BR>>Power = sum(|A,,i,,-B,,i,,|^2^), i=1..N,,subj,, ~ Chi^2^(N,,subj,,) * '''A = B''': Parametric or non-parametric * '''First-level statistic''': Normalized subject averages (not rectified). <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Average: Single subject'' to obtain two averages per subject: A,,i,, and B,,i,,. * '''Second-level statistic''': Non-parametric two-sample t-test, paired, two-tailed. * Mostly applies if all the subjects are sharing the same template anatomy. <<BR>>Not recommended when using individual anatomies because of the sign issue between subjects (the signs might be opposed between two subjects, and the projection of non-rectified values to a template might be inaccurate). * '''|A| = |B|''': Non-parametric * '''First-level statistic''': Rectified and normalized subject averages. <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Average: Group analysis'' to obtain two averages per subject: |A,,i,,| and |B,,i,,|. * '''Second-level statistic''': Non-parametric two-sample t-test, paired, two-tailed. * Non recommended because it does not consider the sign difference within a subject. * '''Power(A) = 0''': Parametric * '''First-level statistic''': Rectified and normalized subject averages. <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Average: Group analysis'' to obtain one average per subject #i: |A,,i,,|. * '''Second-level statistic''': Parametric Chi^2^-test. <<BR>>PowerA = sum(A,,i,,^2^), i=1..N,,subj,, ~ Chi^2^(N,,subj,,). * This tests if the power has increased from baseline. === Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects [???] === * '''|G1| = |G2|''': Non-parametric * '''First-level statistic''': Rectified and normalized subject averages. <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Average: Group analysis'' to obtain one average per subject. * '''Second-level statistic''': Non-parametric two-sample t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. * '''Power(G1) = Power(G2)''': Parametric '''[???]''' * '''First-level statistic''': Rectified and normalized subject averages. <<BR>>Proceed as in ''Average: Group analysis'' to obtain one average per subject: |Ai|. * '''Second-level statistic''': Parametric F-test. <<BR>>PowerG1 = sum(A,,i,,^2^), i=1..N,,1,, ~ Chi^2^(N,,1,,)<<BR>>PowerG2 = sum(Aj^2^), j=1..N,,2,, ~ Chi^2^(N,,2,,)<<BR>>F(N,,1,,,N,,2,,) = PowerG1 / PowerG2 == Unconstrained cortical sources [???] == Three values for each grid point, corresponding to the three dipoles orientations (X,Y,Z). <<BR>>We want only one statistic and one p-value per grid point in output. === Average: Single subject [???] === 1. '''Sensor average''': Compute one sensor-level average''' '''per acquisition run and per condition. 1. '''Sources''': Estimate sources for each average (unconstrained, no normalization). 1. '''Source average''': Average the source-level run averages to get one subject average. 1. '''Low-pass filter''' your evoked responses (optional). 1. '''Normalize '''the subject min-norm averages: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value).<<BR>>'''[???]''' HOW TO NORMALIZE UNCONSTRAINED MAPS WRT BASELINE? === Average: Group analysis [???] === 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above. 1. '''Flatten''' the cortical map: compute the norm of the three orientations at each grid point. 1. '''Project '''the individual source maps on a template (only when using the individual brains). |
Line 108: | Line 204: |
=== Between-subject difference of average === 1. '''Subject averages''': For each subject, compute the within-subject averages for conditions A and B, as described just above. |
=== Difference of averages: Within subject [???] === 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for conditions A and B, as described above. |
Line 111: | Line 207: |
1. '''Rectify''' the difference of source maps (apply an absolute value). | 1. '''Flatten''' the cortical map: compute the norm of the three orientations at each grid point. |
Line 113: | Line 209: |
1. '''Smooth '''spatially the sources. 1. '''Group average''': Compute grand averages of all the subjects. === Within-subject statistics === 1. '''Sources''': Compute source maps for each trial (constrained or unconstrained, no normalization) 1. '''Statistics''': Compare all the trials of condition A vs all the trials of condition B.<<BR>>Use as many trials as possible for A and B: No need to have an equal number of trials. 1. '''A = B''' * '''Parametric''' or '''non-parametric''' tests, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Correct effect size: We identify correctly where and when the conditions are different. * Ambiguous sign: We cannot say which condition has the stronger response. |
1. '''Group average''': Compute grand averages of all the subjects: average_subjects(|Ai-Bi|). === Difference of averages: Between subjects [???] === 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for conditions A and B, as described above. 1. '''Grand averages''': Compute the group-level averages for groups #1 and #2 as described in "Average: Group analysis" 1. '''Difference''': Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(|G1|)-avg(|G2|) 1. '''Limitations''': Because we rectify the source maps before computing the difference, we lose the ability to detect the differences between equal values of opposite signs. And we cannot keep the sign because we are averaging across subjects. Therefore, many effects are not detected correctly. === Statistics: Single subject [???] === 1. '''Sources''': Compute source maps for each trial (unconstrained, no normalization) 1. '''Statistics''': Compare all the trials of condition A vs all the trials of condition B. |
Line 125: | Line 221: |
* '''Non-parametric''' tests only, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Incorrect effect size: Doesn't detect correctly the effects when A and B have opposite signs. * Correct sign: We can identify correctly which condition has a stronger response. === Between-subject statistics === 1. '''Sources''': Compute source maps for each trial (constrained or unconstrained, no normalization) 1. '''First-level statistic''': Compute a t-statistic for the source maps of all the trials A vs. all the trials B. * Process2: "Test > Parametric test: Independent": t-test with equal variance * Use as many trials as possible for A and B: No need to have an equal number of trials. * With a relatively high number of trials, we can consider the t-values follow a Z-distribution. 1. '''Second-level statistic''': Compute a one-sampled power test based on the subject t-statistic. * Process1: "Test > Parametric test against zero": One-sampled Chi-square test * This tests for '''|A-B|'''=0 using a power test: X = sum(|ti|^2) ~ Chi-square distribution * Correct effect size, no sign (cannot detect which condition has the strongest response). === Design considerations === * Use within-subject designs whenever possible (i.e. collect two conditions A and B for each subject), then contrast data within subject before comparing data between subjects. Such designs are not only statistically optimal, but also ameliorate the between-subject sign ambiguities as contrasts can be constructed within each subject. == Unconstrained cortical sources == == Regions of interest (scouts) == |
* '''Non-parametric''' tests only, '''independent''', test norm, two-tailed. * Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect). === Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???] === * '''|A - B| = 0 ''': Non-parametric 1. '''Rectified differences''': Proceed as described in ''Difference of averages: Between subjects'', but stop before the computation of the grand averages (#6) and compute a test instead.<<BR>>You obtain one |A<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>>-B<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>>| value for each subject, test these values against zero. 1. '''Non-parametric''' one-sample test, one-tailed. 1. Indicates when and where there is a significant effect (but not in which direction). * '''|A| = |B|''': Non-parametric 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for A and B, as described above.<<BR>>You obtain two averages per subject (A<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>> and B<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>>). 1. '''Non-parametric''' two-sample test, '''paired''', test absolute values, two-tailed. 1. Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect). === Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects [???] === * '''|A| = |B|''' * '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for A and B, as described above.<<BR>>You obtain two averages per subject (A<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>> and B<<HTML(<SUB>)>>i<<HTML(</SUB>)>>). * '''Non-parametric''' two-sample test, '''independent''', test absolute values, two-tailed. * Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect). == Regions of interest (scouts) [???] == === Statistics: Single subject [???] === |
Line 147: | Line 244: |
== Within-subject statistics == For one unique subject, test for significant differences between two experimental conditions: * Compare the '''single trials''' corresponding to each condition. * In most cases, you '''do not need to normalize''' the data. * For help with the implications of testing the '''relative or absolute values''', see: [[Tutorials/Difference|Difference]]. ==== Unconstrained source maps ==== * Three values per vertex. * Use the non-normalized minimum norm maps for all the trials (current density maps, no Z-score). * We need to test the '''norm '''of the three orientations instead of testing the orientations separately. * '''Norm(A) vs. Norm(B)''': * Null hypothesis H0: (|A|=|B|). * '''Non-parametric''' tests only, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Incorrect effect size, meaningful sign. ==== Regions of interest (scouts) ==== * Average/constrast cortical maps before summarizing scout activity. |
|
Line 168: | Line 246: |
==== Time-frequency maps ==== * Test the non-normalized time-frequency maps for all the trials (no Z-score or ERS/ERD). * The values tested are power or magnitudes, all positive, so (A=B) and (|A|=|B|) are equivalent. * '''|A| vs |B|''': * Null hypothesis H0: (|A|=|B|) * '''Non-parametric''' tests only, independent, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * Correct effect size, meaningful sign. == Between-subject statistics [TODO] == ==== Subject averages ==== You need first to process the data separately for each subject: 1. Sources computed on individual brains: '''Project '''the individual source maps on a template (see the [[Tutorials/CoregisterSubjects|coregistration tutorial]]). Not needed if the sources were estimated directly on the template anatomy. <<BR>>Note: We evaluated the alternative order (project the sources and then normalize): it doesn't seem to be making a significant difference. It's more practical then to normalize at the subject level before projecting the sources on the template, so that we have normalized maps to look at for each subject in the database. 1. Constrained sources: '''Smooth '''spatially the sources, to make sure the brain responses are aligned. '''Problem''': This is only possible after applying an absolute value, smoothing in relative values do not make sense, as the positive and negative signals and the two sides of a sulcus would cancel out. [TODO] ==== Paired tests ==== * '''Constrained source maps''' (one value per vertex): * '''(A-B=0)''': Parametric or non-parametric tests, two-tailed, FDR-corrected ('''sign issue?'''). * '''(|A|-|B|=0)''': Non-parametric tests, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * '''(|A-B| = 0)''': ??? * '''Unconstrained source maps''' (three values per vertex): * '''(Norm(A-B)=0)''': Non-parametric tests, __'''one-tailed'''__ (non-negative statistic), FDR-corrected. * '''(Norm(A)-Norm(B)=0)''': Non-parametric tests, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * '''Time-frequency maps''': * '''(|A|-|B|=0)''': Non-parametric tests, two-tailed, FDR-corrected. * '''Regions of interest''' (scouts): * Comparison of scout time series between subjects is tricky because there is no way to avoid sign ambiguity for different subjects. Thus there are no clear recommendations. Rectifying before comparing scout time series between subjects can be a good idea or not depending on different cases. Having a good understanding of the data (multiple inspections across channels/sources/subjects) can offer hints whether rectifying the scout time series is a good idea. Using unconstrained cortical maps to create the scout time series can ameliorate ambiguity concerns. * For help with '''relative/absolute options''', read the previous tutorial: [[Tutorials/Difference|Difference]]. ==== Averages ==== * In order to compute grand averages (across subjects), you should '''rectify''' your source maps before averaging. Averaging the absolute values of the subject-level averages will help avoiding possible cancellation effects due to anatomical differences between subjects. * If you have two conditions A and B to contrast, first compute the difference within-subject (A-B), then average the rectified differences: average_subjects(|Ai-Bi|). |
=== Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???] === * Comparison of scout time series between subjects is tricky because there is no way to avoid sign ambiguity for different subjects. Thus there are no clear recommendations. Rectifying before comparing scout time series between subjects can be a good idea or not depending on different cases. Having a good understanding of the data (multiple inspections across channels/sources/subjects) can offer hints whether rectifying the scout time series is a good idea. Using unconstrained cortical maps to create the scout time series can ameliorate ambiguity concerns. == Time-frequency maps [???] == === Average: Single subject [???] === 1. '''Time-frequency maps''': Compute time-frequency maps for each trial. * Apply the default measure: magnitude for Hilbert transform, power for Morlet wavelets. * Do not normalize the source maps: no Z-score or ERS/ERD. * The values are all strictly positive, there is no sign ambiguity as for recordings or sources. 1. '''Average''' all the time-frequency maps together, for each condition separately. * If you are averaging time-frequency maps computed on sensor-level data, the same limitations apply as for averaging sensor level data (see sections about MEG and EEG recordings above). === Average: Group analysis [???] === 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above. 1. '''Normalize''': '''[???]''' Zscore, ERD/ERS, or FieldTrip?<<BR>>Justification: The amplitude range of current densities may vary between subjects because of anatomical or experimental differences. This normalization helps bringing the different subjects to the same range of values. 1. '''Group average''': Compute grand averages of all the subjects. === Difference of averages [???] === 1. '''Group average''': Compute the averages for conditions A and B as in ''Average: Group analysis''. 1. '''Difference''': Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(A)-avg(B). === Statistics: Single subject [???] === 1. '''Time-frequency maps''': Compute time-frequency maps for each trial. * Apply the default measure: magnitude for Hilbert transform, power for Morlet wavelets. * Do not normalize the source maps: no Z-score or ERS/ERD. * The values are all strictly positive, there is no sign ambiguity as for recordings or sources. 1. '''Statistics''': Compare all the trials of condition A vs all the trials of condition B. 1. '''A = B''' '''[???]''' * '''Parametric''' or '''non-parametric''' t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. '''[???]''' * Indicates both where there is a significant effect and what is its direction (no sign ambiguity). === Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???] === * '''A = B ''''''[???]''' 1. '''Subject averages''': Compute within-subject averages for all subjects, as described above. 1. '''Parametric''' or '''non-parametric''' t-test, '''independent''', two-tailed. '''[???]''' 1. Indicates both where there is a significant effect and what is its direction (no sign ambiguity). |
Line 210: | Line 291: |
* [Group analysis] Unconstrained sources: How to compute a Z-score? * Zscore(A): Normalizes each orientation separately, which doesn't make much sense. * Zscore(Norm(A)): Gets rid of the signs, forbids the option of a signed test H0:(Norm(A-B)=0) |
* [Group analysis] Unconstrained sources: How to normalize wrt baseline with a Z-score? * Zscore(A): Normalizes each orientation separately, we cannot take the norm of it after. * Zscore(|A|): Gets rid of the signs, forbids the option of a signed test H0:(Norm(A-B)=0) |
Line 214: | Line 295: |
* We would need a way to normalize across the three orientations are the same time. * [Group analysis] Constrained sources: How do we smooth? * Group analysis benefits a lot from smoothing the source maps before computing statistics. * However this requires to apply an absolute value first. How do we do? |
* We need a way to normalize across the three orientations are the same time. |
Line 219: | Line 297: |
* Norm(A)-Norm(B): Cannot detect correctly the differences * (A-B): We test individually each orientation, which doesn't make much sense. * We would need a test for the three orientations at once. * [Group analysis] Rectify source maps? * Recommended in Dimitrios' guidelines, which is incoherent with the rest of the page. |
* |A|-|B|: Cannot detect correctly the difference. * |A-B|: Cannot be computed because the trials are not paired. * We need a test for the three orientations at the same time. * [Group analysis] Unconstrained sources:Can we use parametric tests? * Time-frequency maps: * Can we use parametric tests for (A-B=0) ? Does (A-B) ~ normal distribution? * Do we need to normalize the time-frequency maps when testing across subjects? * If yes, how to normalize the time-frequency maps? (Z-score, ERS/ERD, divide by std) |
Tutorial 27: Workflows
[TUTORIAL UNDER DEVELOPMENT: NOT READY FOR PUBLIC USE]
Authors: Francois Tadel, Elizabeth Bock, Dimitrios Pantazis, Richard Leahy, Sylvain Baillet
This page provides some general recommendations for your event-related analysis. It is not directly related with the auditory dataset, but provides guidelines you should consider for any MEG/EEG experiment.
We do not provide standard analysis pipelines for resting or steady state recordings yet, but we will add a few examples soon in the section Other analysis scenarios of the tutorials page.
Contents
What is your question?
The most appropriate analysis pipeline for your data depends on the question you are trying to answer. Before defining what are the main steps of your analysis, you should be able to state clearly the question you want to answer with your recordings.
What dimension?
- MEG/EEG recordings
- Cortical sources
- Individual anatomy or template
- Constrained (one value per vertex) or unconstrained (three values per grid point)
- Full cortex or regions of interests
- Frequency or time-frequency maps
What kind of experiment?
Single subject: Contrast two experimental conditions across trials, for one single subject.
- Files A: Single trials for condition A.
- Files B: Single trials for condition B.
Group analysis, within subject: Contrast two conditions A and B measured for each subject.
- Files A: Subject-level averages for condition A (all the subjects).
- Files B: Subject-level averages for condition B (all the subjects).
Group analysis, between subjects: Contrast two groups of subjects for one condition.
- Files A: Subject-level averages for group #1 (G1).
- Files B: Subject-level averages for group #2 (G2).
What level of precision?
- Difference of averages
- Statistically significant differences between conditions or groups
What statistical test?
A = B
Tests the null hypothesis H0:(A=B) against the alternative hypothesis H1:(A≠B)
Correct detection: Identify correctly where and when the conditions are different.
- Ambiguous sign: We cannot say which condition is stronger.
Power(A) = Power(B)
Tests the null hypothesis H0:(Power(A)=Power(B)) against the alternative hypothesis H1:(Power(A)≠Power(B))
- Incorrect detection: Not sensitive to the cases where A and B have opposite signs.
Meaningful sign: We can identify correctly which condition has a stronger response.
Power(x) = |x|2, where |x| represents the modulus of the values:
- Absolute value for scalar values (recordings, constrained sources, time-frequency)
- Norm of the three orientations for unconstrained sources.
Multiple comparisons: FDR is a good choice for correcting p-values for multiple comparisons.
Design considerations
- Use within-subject designs whenever possible (i.e. collect two conditions A and B for each subject), then contrast data at the subject level before comparing data between subjects.
- Such designs are not only statistically optimal, but also ameliorate the between-subject sign ambiguities as contrasts can be constructed within each subject.
Common pre-processing pipeline
Most event-related studies can start with the pipeline we've introduced in these tutorials.
- Import the anatomy of the subject (or use a template for all the subjects).
- Access the recordings:
- Link the continuous recordings to the Brainstorm database.
- Prepare the channel file: co-register sensors and MRI, edit type and name of channels.
- Edit the event markers: fix the delays of the triggers, mark additional events.
- Pre-process the signals:
- Evaluate the quality of the recordings with a power spectral density plot (PSD).
- Apply frequency filters (low-pass, high-pass, notch).
- Identify bad channels and bad segments.
- Correct for artifacts with SSP or ICA.
- Import the recordings in the database: epochs around some markers of interest.
How many trials to include?
Single subject: Include all the good trials (unless you have a very low number of trials).
See the averaging tutorial.Group analysis: Use a similar numbers of trials for all the subjects (no need to be strictly equal), reject the subjects for which you have much less good trials.
EEG recordings
Average
- Average the epochs across acquisition runs: OK.
- Average the epochs across subjects: OK.
- Electrodes are in the same standard positions for all the subjects (e.g. 10-20).
- Never use an absolute value for averaging or contrasting sensor-level data.
Statistics: Single subject
A = B: Parametric or non-parametric t-test, independent, two-tailed.
Statistics: Group analysis, within subject
A = B
First-level statistic: For each subject, sensor average for conditions A and B.
Second-level statistic: Parametric or non-parametric t-test, paired, two-tailed.
Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects
A = B
First-level statistic: For each subject, sensor average for the conditions to test.
Second-level statistic: Parametric/non-parametric t-test, independent, two-tailed.
MEG recordings
Average
- Average the epochs within each acquisition runs: OK.
- Average across runs: Not advised because the head of the subject may move between runs.
- Average across subjects: Strongly discouraged because the shape of the heads vary but the sensors are fixed. One sensor does not correspond to the same brain region for different subjects.
- Tolerance for data exploration: Averaging across runs and subjects can be useful for identifying time points and sensors with interesting effects but should be avoided for formal analysis.
- Note for Elekta/MaxFilter users: You can align all acquisition run to a reference run, this will allow direct channel comparisons and averaging across runs. Not recommended across subjects.
- Never use an absolute value for averaging or contrasting sensor-level data.
Statistics: Single subject
A = B: Parametric or non-parametric t-test, independent, two-tailed.
Statistics: Group analysis
- Not recommended with MEG recordings: do your analysis in source space.
Constrained cortical sources
Average: Single subject
Sensor average: Compute one sensor-level average per acquisition run and per condition.
Sources: Estimate sources for each average (constrained, no normalization).
Source average: Average the source-level run averages to get one subject average.
Compute a weighted average to balance for different numbers of trials across runs.Low-pass filter your evoked responses (optional).
If you filter the average before normalizing wrt baseline, it will lead to an underestimation of the baseline variance, and therefore to an overestimation of the Z scores computed in the next step, especially if the baseline is too short (typically less than 200 time points). The filter increases the autocorrelation of the time series, and therefore biases the estimation of the signal variance (Wikipedia).Normalize the subject min-norm averages: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value).
Justification: The amplitude range of current densities may vary between subjects because of anatomical or experimental differences. This normalization helps bringing the different subjects to the same range of values.Do not rectify the cortical maps, but display them as absolute values if needed.
Average: Group analysis
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above.
Rectify the cortical maps (apply an absolute value).
Justification: Cortical maps have ambiguous signs across subjects: reconstructed sources depend heavily on the orientation of true cortical sources. Given the folding patterns of individual cortical anatomies vary considerably, cortical maps have subject-specific amplitude and sign ambiguities. This is true even if a standard anatomy is used for reconstruction.Project the individual source maps on a template (only when using the individual brains).
For more details, see tutorial: Group analysis: Subject coregistration.Group average: Compute grand averages of all the subjects.
Do not use a weighted average: all the subjects should have the same weight in this average.Smooth spatially the source maps (optional).
You can smooth after step #3 for computing non-parametric statistics with the subject averages. For a simple group average, it is equivalent to smooth before of after computing the average.
Difference of averages: Within subject
Sensor average: Compute one sensor-level average per acquisition run and condition.
Sources: Estimate sources for each average (constrained, no normalization).
Source average: Average the source-level session averages to get one subject average.
Subject difference: Compute the difference between conditions for each subject #i: (Ai-Bi)
Low-pass filter the difference (optional)
Normalize the difference: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value): Z(Ai-Bi)
Rectify the difference (apply an absolute value): |Z(Ai-Bi)|
Project the individual difference on a template (only when using the individual brains).
Group average: Compute grand averages of all the subjects: avg(|Z(Ai-Bi)|).
Smooth spatially the source maps (optional).
Difference of averages: Between subjects
Grand averages: Compute averages for groups #1 and #2 as in Average:Group analysis.
Difference: Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(|G1|)-avg(|G2|)
Limitations: Because we rectify the source maps before computing the difference, we lose the ability to detect the differences between equal values of opposite signs. And we cannot keep the sign because we are averaging across subjects. Therefore, many effects are not detected correctly.
Statistics: Single subject
A = B: Parametric or non-parametric
- Compute source maps for each trial (constrained, no normalization).
Parametric or non-parametric two-sample t-test, two-tailed.
Identifies correctly where and when the conditions are different (sign not meaningful).Directionality: Additional step to know which condition has higher values.
Compute the difference of rectified averages: |avg(Ai)|-|avg(Bi)|
Combine the significance level (t-test) with the direction (difference): See details.
Power(A) = Power(B): Parametric [???]
Compute source maps for each trial (constrained, dSPM).
Parametric F-test: Trials Ai and Bi must be normalized (zero mean, unit variance)
PowerA = sum(Ai2)/NA ~ Chi2(NA), PowerB = sum(Bj2)/NB ~ Chi2(NB)
F = PowerA / Power ~ F(NA,NB), can be tested with the F-distribution.
Power(A) = 0: Parametric [???]
- Power(s(t)) = sum_trials (s(t)^2);
Power_baseline = mean_{t<0} (sum_trials(s_base(t)^2));
- F(large,large) = Power(s(t)) / Power_baseline;
But more popular statistic: Power(s(t) – power(baseline) / power(baseline)
Do not have parametric statistic
Statistics: Group analysis, within subject
Power(A-B) = 0: Parametric
First-level statistic: Rectified difference of normalized averages.
Proceed as in Difference of averages: Within subjects, but stop before the group average (after step #8). You obtain one measure |Ai-Bi| per subject, test these values against zero.Second-level statistic: Parametric Chi2-test.
Power = sum(|Ai-Bi|2), i=1..Nsubj ~ Chi2(Nsubj)
A = B: Parametric or non-parametric
First-level statistic: Normalized subject averages (not rectified).
Proceed as in Average: Single subject to obtain two averages per subject: Ai and Bi.Second-level statistic: Non-parametric two-sample t-test, paired, two-tailed.
Mostly applies if all the subjects are sharing the same template anatomy.
Not recommended when using individual anatomies because of the sign issue between subjects (the signs might be opposed between two subjects, and the projection of non-rectified values to a template might be inaccurate).
|A| = |B|: Non-parametric
First-level statistic: Rectified and normalized subject averages.
Proceed as in Average: Group analysis to obtain two averages per subject: |Ai| and |Bi|.Second-level statistic: Non-parametric two-sample t-test, paired, two-tailed.
- Non recommended because it does not consider the sign difference within a subject.
Power(A) = 0: Parametric
First-level statistic: Rectified and normalized subject averages.
Proceed as in Average: Group analysis to obtain one average per subject #i: |Ai|.Second-level statistic: Parametric Chi2-test.
PowerA = sum(Ai2), i=1..Nsubj ~ Chi2(Nsubj).- This tests if the power has increased from baseline.
Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects [???]
|G1| = |G2|: Non-parametric
First-level statistic: Rectified and normalized subject averages.
Proceed as in Average: Group analysis to obtain one average per subject.Second-level statistic: Non-parametric two-sample t-test, independent, two-tailed.
Power(G1) = Power(G2): Parametric [???]
First-level statistic: Rectified and normalized subject averages.
Proceed as in Average: Group analysis to obtain one average per subject: |Ai|.Second-level statistic: Parametric F-test.
PowerG1 = sum(Ai2), i=1..N1 ~ Chi2(N1)
PowerG2 = sum(Aj2), j=1..N2 ~ Chi2(N2)
F(N1,N2) = PowerG1 / PowerG2
Unconstrained cortical sources [???]
Three values for each grid point, corresponding to the three dipoles orientations (X,Y,Z).
We want only one statistic and one p-value per grid point in output.
Average: Single subject [???]
Sensor average: Compute one sensor-level average per acquisition run and per condition.
Sources: Estimate sources for each average (unconstrained, no normalization).
Source average: Average the source-level run averages to get one subject average.
Low-pass filter your evoked responses (optional).
Normalize the subject min-norm averages: Z-score wrt baseline (no absolute value).
[???] HOW TO NORMALIZE UNCONSTRAINED MAPS WRT BASELINE?
Average: Group analysis [???]
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above.
Flatten the cortical map: compute the norm of the three orientations at each grid point.
Project the individual source maps on a template (only when using the individual brains).
Group average: Compute grand averages of all the subjects.
Difference of averages: Within subject [???]
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for conditions A and B, as described above.
Subject difference: Compute the difference between conditions for each subject (A-B).
Flatten the cortical map: compute the norm of the three orientations at each grid point.
Project the individual difference on a template.
Group average: Compute grand averages of all the subjects: average_subjects(|Ai-Bi|).
Difference of averages: Between subjects [???]
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for conditions A and B, as described above.
Grand averages: Compute the group-level averages for groups #1 and #2 as described in "Average: Group analysis"
Difference: Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(|G1|)-avg(|G2|)
Limitations: Because we rectify the source maps before computing the difference, we lose the ability to detect the differences between equal values of opposite signs. And we cannot keep the sign because we are averaging across subjects. Therefore, many effects are not detected correctly.
Statistics: Single subject [???]
Sources: Compute source maps for each trial (unconstrained, no normalization)
Statistics: Compare all the trials of condition A vs all the trials of condition B.
|A| = |B|
Non-parametric tests only, independent, test norm, two-tailed.
- Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect).
Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???]
|A - B| = 0 : Non-parametric
Rectified differences: Proceed as described in Difference of averages: Between subjects, but stop before the computation of the grand averages (#6) and compute a test instead.
You obtain one |Ai-Bi| value for each subject, test these values against zero.Non-parametric one-sample test, one-tailed.
- Indicates when and where there is a significant effect (but not in which direction).
|A| = |B|: Non-parametric
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for A and B, as described above.
You obtain two averages per subject (Ai and Bi).Non-parametric two-sample test, paired, test absolute values, two-tailed.
- Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect).
Statistics: Group analysis, between subjects [???]
|A| = |B|
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for A and B, as described above.
You obtain two averages per subject (Ai and Bi).Non-parametric two-sample test, independent, test absolute values, two-tailed.
- Indicates which condition corresponds to a stronger brain response (for a known effect).
Regions of interest (scouts) [???]
Statistics: Single subject [???]
- Even within-subject cortical maps have sign ambiguities. MEG has limited spatial resolution and sources in opposing sulcal/gyral areas are reconstructed with inverted signs (constrained orientations only). Averaging activity in cortical regions of interest (scouts) would thus lead to signal cancelation. To avoid this brainstorm uses algorithms to manipulate the sign of individual sources before averaging within a cortical region. Unfortunately, this introduces an amplitude and sign ambiguity in the time course when summarizing scout activity.
As a result, perform any interesting within-subject average/contrast before computing an average scout time series.
- Then consider as constrained or unconstrained source maps.
Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???]
- Comparison of scout time series between subjects is tricky because there is no way to avoid sign ambiguity for different subjects. Thus there are no clear recommendations. Rectifying before comparing scout time series between subjects can be a good idea or not depending on different cases. Having a good understanding of the data (multiple inspections across channels/sources/subjects) can offer hints whether rectifying the scout time series is a good idea. Using unconstrained cortical maps to create the scout time series can ameliorate ambiguity concerns.
Time-frequency maps [???]
Average: Single subject [???]
Time-frequency maps: Compute time-frequency maps for each trial.
- Apply the default measure: magnitude for Hilbert transform, power for Morlet wavelets.
- Do not normalize the source maps: no Z-score or ERS/ERD.
- The values are all strictly positive, there is no sign ambiguity as for recordings or sources.
Average all the time-frequency maps together, for each condition separately.
- If you are averaging time-frequency maps computed on sensor-level data, the same limitations apply as for averaging sensor level data (see sections about MEG and EEG recordings above).
Average: Group analysis [???]
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for all the subjects, as described above.
Normalize: [???] Zscore, ERD/ERS, or FieldTrip?
Justification: The amplitude range of current densities may vary between subjects because of anatomical or experimental differences. This normalization helps bringing the different subjects to the same range of values.Group average: Compute grand averages of all the subjects.
Difference of averages [???]
Group average: Compute the averages for conditions A and B as in Average: Group analysis.
Difference: Compute the difference between group-level averages: avg(A)-avg(B).
Statistics: Single subject [???]
Time-frequency maps: Compute time-frequency maps for each trial.
- Apply the default measure: magnitude for Hilbert transform, power for Morlet wavelets.
- Do not normalize the source maps: no Z-score or ERS/ERD.
- The values are all strictly positive, there is no sign ambiguity as for recordings or sources.
Statistics: Compare all the trials of condition A vs all the trials of condition B.
A = B [???]
Parametric or non-parametric t-test, independent, two-tailed. [???]
- Indicates both where there is a significant effect and what is its direction (no sign ambiguity).
Statistics: Group analysis, within subject [???]
A = B [???]
Subject averages: Compute within-subject averages for all subjects, as described above.
Parametric or non-parametric t-test, independent, two-tailed. [???]
- Indicates both where there is a significant effect and what is its direction (no sign ambiguity).
Workflow: Current problems [TODO]
The following inconsistencies are still present in the documentation. We are actively working on these issues and will update this tutorial as soon as we found solutions.
- [Group analysis] Unconstrained sources: How to normalize wrt baseline with a Z-score?
- Zscore(A): Normalizes each orientation separately, we cannot take the norm of it after.
- Zscore(|A|): Gets rid of the signs, forbids the option of a signed test H0:(Norm(A-B)=0)
See also the tutorial: Source estimation
- We need a way to normalize across the three orientations are the same time.
- [Single subject] Unconstrained sources: How do compare two conditions with multiple trials?
- |A|-|B|: Cannot detect correctly the difference.
- |A-B|: Cannot be computed because the trials are not paired.
- We need a test for the three orientations at the same time.
- [Group analysis] Unconstrained sources:Can we use parametric tests?
- Time-frequency maps:
- Can we use parametric tests for (A-B=0) ? Does (A-B) ~ normal distribution?
- Do we need to normalize the time-frequency maps when testing across subjects?
- If yes, how to normalize the time-frequency maps? (Z-score, ERS/ERD, divide by std)